The European warning services (eaws) have agreed on the following avalanche problems and now also use standardized symbols: Fresh snow, drifting snow, wet snow, old snow, sliding snow
In addition, there is an icon for "favorable situation" (explanation of avalanche problems). In North America, other problems are sometimes named, such as cornice breakage, loose snow (wet and dry), as well as "deep slab" and "persistent slab". The latter are summarized in Europe as "old snow".
Drew Hardesty has worked intensively on avalanche problems and, together with his colleague Wendy Wagner, has developed the "Avalanche Toolbox". In the toolbox you can see schematically which "tool" is appropriate for which problem. (See also) Three factors are considered for each problem: Predictability of "behavior", probability of remote triggers, usefulness of snow observations and tests. In this way, regardless of the hazard level, the difference between old snow problems and, for example, a new snow situation is worked out.
In the following commentary, Hardesty explains more about avalanche problems and the difficulty of summarizing complex snowpack situations in a hazard level. The text originally appeared on the Utah Avalanche Center blog and has been translated and lightly edited by PG in consultation with the author:
The Danger in the Danger Levels
"Democracy is the worst of all forms of government, except for all others." Who said that again? Mark Twain? No, Winston Churchill. But it sounds like he stole it from someone (Twain?). It's a bit like democracy with our avalanche danger levels.
Recently, at a lecture, I asked the participants to determine the danger level in two different situations. Beforehand, I gave them a little crash course: When we write our situation report, we evaluate each potential avalanche problem based on a number of factors:
The likelihood of triggering
Bruce Jamieson did a study a few years ago where he asked experts (avalanche watchers and the like) about the likelihood of triggering at different danger levels. (See illustration). The probability of triggering increases approximately 10-fold with an increase in the danger level.
In the past, the factor "probability of triggering" was considered the most important point in determining the danger level. Terms such as "unlikely", "possible", "probable" played a major role. As a rule, the level was indicated as significant when self-triggering came within the realm of possibility. This system did not work well. Imagine someone offered you 10 million dollars to play Russian roulette. Would it influence your decision if you knew how many rounds were in the magazine? What if I told you it had a rubber bullet in it? A bullet? A cannonball? A nuclear missile? Would you change your mind?
I asked my lecture participants to estimate the probability of triggering per danger level. The result was something like:
Low - 1-5%
Moderate - 25-35%
Significant - 60-65%
Large - 75-85%
Very large - 90-100%
I found the jump from moderate (35%) to significant (60%) particularly interesting. What is the key difference between these two levels? After all, most people are at levels 2 and 3. Where did the missing 25% go?